Stronger reaction to masculinity threats tied to testosterone, Stanford sociologist says
Professor of sociology Robb Willer says men overcompensate when their masculinity is threatened. Willer's new research suggests that the higher the man's testosterone level, the stronger the reaction.
BY BROOKE DONALD
Men with higher levels of testosterone are more easily threatened, engaging in masculine overcompensation in response, says a Stanford sociologist.
We've all heard it before: if a guy is made to feel less than manly, he'll act even more macho to make up for it.
Now, new research suggests that this behavior may have something to do with how much testosterone a man has.
Robb Willer, an associate professor of sociology at Stanford, has been studying masculinity since he was a graduate student, lending empirical data to the popular beliefs about emasculation.
In one of his first inquiries into the subject, back in 2005, he found that men whose masculinity is threatened express more masculine traits and behaviors. For example, Willer has found that threatened men show stronger support for war and greater interest in buying an SUV – attitudes and behaviors culturally associated with masculinity.
Willer's new research follows up on that earlier study, finding that men higher in testosterone are more easily threatened, engaging in masculine overcompensation in response.
Willer describes the results in "Overdoing Gender: A Test of the Masculine Overcompensation Thesis," published in theAmerican Journal of Sociology.
The paper includes four studies examining the theory of male overcompensation – that men react to the undermining of their masculine status with bold demonstrations of strength and masculinity. The most recent study investigates testosterone’s role.
To conduct the study, researchers enlisted 54 undergraduate men at the University of Iowa. The men took a gender identity survey in which they were asked questions about their assertiveness or other qualities typically associated with masculinity or femininity.
They were then given feedback that the men believed was based on the survey but in fact was random. Some of the men were told their answers indicated more feminine traits. Others were told the answers indicated more masculine behavior. Then they were surveyed again on a variety of political and cultural views.
The men’s saliva was collected at different points in the study allowing their testosterone levels to be measured.
The research revealed that men with higher levels of testosterone show the strongest overcompensation response, Willer said. Higher testosterone men showed greater increases on masculine attitudes like support for war and negative views of homosexuality after being told their answers indicated more feminine traits. Lower testosterone men showed no effects when similarly threatened.
"Masculine overcompensation in men appears to be driven by men with moderate to high testosterone levels," Willer said. "Their levels of support for war and homophobia practically doubled on the scale that we measured them on, where lower testosterone men were unaffected by threats."
Willer said the results do not suggest that men with higher testosterone levels are always more aggressive or dominant. The relationship between testosterone and macho behavior shows itself when triggered by a threat or provocation, Willer said.
"There appears to be a very strong effect when a high testosterone man's status is threatened," Willer said. “However, when not threatened, we saw no differences in the attitudes of men with different testosterone levels.”
Willer said there is still more to be done to unravel the reasons men overcompensate, including whether it's behavior to reassure themselves or prove to society their manliness. He said future studies should also look cross-culturally, and examine whether the same theory could materialize for women in certain contexts.
Co-authors include Christabel L. Rogalin of Purdue University, North Central; Bridget Conlon of California State University, Fresno; and Michael T. Wojnowicz of the University of Washington.
Media Contact
- Robb Willer, Sociology, willer@stanford.edu
- Brooke Donald, Stanford News Service: (650) 725-0224, brooke.donald@stanford.edu
Friday, April 19, 2013
Men Higher in Testosterone Are More Easily Threatened, Respond with Masculine Overcompensation
This press release from Stanford University highlights some new research showing that men with higher testosterone levels are more likely to feel their masculinity threatened. They tend to respond with overcompensating displays of masculinity. Clearly, this does not apply to all men, but it is a broad generalization that applies to a lot of men.
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment